Developmental Trends in Children’s Use of Emotion Terms

نویسندگان

  • Sherri C. Widen
  • James A. Russell
چکیده

Children (N = 64; 2 and 3 years of age) labeled another child’s emotion, based on either a prototypical facial expression or on a story about prototypical emotional events for 5 emotions (happiness, sadness, anger, fear, and disgust). Proportion correct was low and improved only gradually with age. Children shown the faces produced more correct labels than did those told the stories. Initially, children used (and overgeneralized) two emotion labels (happy and sad) and only gradually added others (anger, fear, and disgust, in that order). Introduction Told that Judd is receiving presents at his birthday party, most adults would guess that Judd is happy. Shown Judd smiling, most adults would, again, guess that Judd is happy. But which of these two cues – a story about the emotion’s antecedents and consequences or a picture of its facial expression – is the better cue? Which cue is better for preschool children? An answer to this question can help chart the development of children’s emotion categories underlying their use of emotion terms. One possibility is that to a preschooler facial expression is a primary source of information about the emotions of others (Izard, 1994; Denham, 1998; Magai & McFadden, 1995). If so, young children might do better (make fewer errors) with facial expressions than with other cues to emotion. Another possibility is that causal antecedents and consequences are a primary source of information in that children understand emotion in a stimulus-response framework (Bullock & Russell, 1986; Harris, 1983). If so, young children might do better with stories that tell of the event that leads to the emotion. The cue that works best for younger children is likely to be the origin of children’s understanding of emotion. In this study, we asked children, aged 2 or 3 years, what emotion Judd is feeling, given one of two cues: either a prototypical facial expression of the emotion or a story specifying a prototypical causal antecedent events to the emotion and its consequences. We examined five emotions (happiness, fear, anger, disgust, sadness). Method Participants Participants were 64 children (32 girls and 32 boys) enrolled in daycares and preschools in or near Vancouver, British Columbia. All children were proficient in English. There were 16 children (8 girls and 8 boys) in each of four age groups: young 2s (24 to 29 months), older 2s (30 to 35 months), young 3s (36 to 41 months), and older 3s (42 to 47 months). Materials Photographs of Facial Expressions. A set of 5 black and white 5 x 7” photographs of a 12-year-old boy posing of prototypical facial expressions of emotion (happiness, fear, anger, disgust, sadness) were used (Widen & Russell, 2001). The facial expressions had been prepared to meet Ekman and Friesen’s (1978) criteria for particular discrete emotions. Stories of Emotional Events. Five stories describing stereotypical emotioneliciting events and responses were created (Table 1) based on prior work in our lab in which children generated causes and consequences of specific emotions. The children were shown a drawing depicting a setting of each story (e.g., a bedroom) while the story was being read. Procedure Each child participated in the two parts of the procedure in a single session. Each child’s emotion concepts were first primed. The child was then randomly assigned to either the face or story condition. The child was asked to label either five facial expressions or five stories of emotional events. Priming. The experimenter first spent time playing with a child until the child seemed comfortable with the experimenter. In order to prime the child’s emotion concepts, the experimenter began a conversation about whether the child and his or her parents had ever felt the five target emotions (happy, sad, mad, scared, disgusted). The experimenter asked, for example, “Does Y ever feel happy?” “Do you sometimes feel mad?” Every effort was made throughout the experiment to use a neutral tone of voice when presenting the emotion words. Faces. The experimenter introduced the faces by saying, “I brought some pictures of Judd a boy named Judd. Would you like to look at them with me? Okay, here is a picture of Judd [showing the neutral expression]. Do you know what Judd is going to do? He is going to show us how he feels sometimes.” The experimenter then showed the child the five facial expressions, one at a time in a random order. For the first face, the experimenter said, “One day, Judd felt like this [pointing to the face].” For the other faces, the experimenter said, “One week later, Judd felt like this [pointing to the picture].” After each picture, the experimenter asked, “How do you think Judd feels in this picture?” Stories. The experimenter introduced the stories (Table 1) by saying, “I’m going to tell you some stories about things that happened to a boy named Judd. After each one, you get to tell me how you think Judd feels. How does that sound? Remember: listen carefully, because you have to tell me how Judd feels.” The experimenter then presented the stories, one at a time in a random order. The first story began, “Once upon a time,” and the other stories began, “One week later...” After each story, the experimenter asked, “How do you think Judd feels?” Scoring The child participants were allowed to use any label they chose. The scoring key used in this study was drawn from Widen and Russell (2001) who describe the development of a scoring key based on ratings of two judges blind to the source of the labels. Responses to the faces and stories that were scored as correct for happiness were happy, excited, going to play; for fear, scared; for disgust, disgusted, yucky, gross; for anger, angry, mad, cross, frustrated, grumpy; and for sad, sad. Responses varied from what was just listed in syntax or by being embedded in a phrase (e.g., very scared, totally grossed out). These were all the labels children used that came close to specifying the specific emotion.

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION Running head: INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION Developmental Differences in Children’s Interpersonal Emotion Regulation

Previous research on interpersonal emotion regulation (ER) in childhood has been rather unsystematic, focusing mainly on children’s prosocial behaviour, and has been conducted in the absence of an integrative emotion theoretical framework. The present research relied on the interpersonal affect classification proposed by Niven, Totterdell, and Holman (2009) to investigate children’s use of diff...

متن کامل

Developmental Aspects of Self-Regulation in Germany and Chile: Links among Maternal Warmth, Children’s Self-Regulation, and Social Competence

The present study investigated relations among maternal warmth, children’s self-regulation (i.e., behavior and emotion regulation), and prosocial behavior. The assumed relations were studied in Germany and Chile, two socialization contexts differing in socio-economic and cultural factors. The sample consisted of 76 German and 167 Chilean fourth graders, their mothers, and teachers. Maternal war...

متن کامل

Temperament theory and the study of cognition–emotion interactions across development

In this paper we review current definitions and measurement approaches used to assess individual differences in children’s temperament. We review the neural bases of temperamental reactivity and self-regulation and propose that these constructs provide a framework for examining individual differences and developmental change in emotion–cognition interactions across development. Studies are revi...

متن کامل

Age-Dependent Positivity-Bias in Children’s Processing of Emotion Terms

Emotions play an important role in human communication, and the daily-life interactions of young children often include situations that require the verbalization of emotional states with verbal means, e.g., with emotion terms. Through them, one can express own emotional states and those of others. Thus, the acquisition of emotion terms allows children to participate more intensively in social c...

متن کامل

Children’s Play as a Context for Managing Physiological Arousal and Learning Emotion Regulation

In this paper I examine children’s play as a context for managing physiological arousal and learning to regulate strong emotions. I define emotion regulation as the process by which children monitor and control their emotional states and their expression to adapt to different social situations or demands. Age trends and gender differences in emotion regulation problems and competencies are desc...

متن کامل

Evaluation of predisposing factors for developmental delay of pre-term infants in the first year of life

Background: Developmental delay in preterm infants was estimated to be more common than term infants. Identifying the factors predisposing to developmental delay can help experts and health professionals in this field to prevent developmental delay of the infants, and leads to better management of the condition of them. This study aimed to evaluate and investigate the predisposing factors of de...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2004